Contents 1 Theories 1.1 Template matching 1.2 Prototype matching 1.3 Feature analysis 1.4 Multiple Discrimination Scaling 1.5 Recognition by Components Theory 1.6 Top-down and Bottom-up Processing 1.6.1 Top-Down Processing 1.6.2 Bottom-up Processing 2 Seriation 2.1 Piaget's Work on Seriation 2.2 Development of Problem Solving Skills 2.3 Application of Seriation in Schools 3 Facial Pattern Recognition 3.1 Neural Mechanisms 3.2 Facial Recognition Development 3.3 Developmental Issues 4 Language Development 4.1 Pattern Recognition in Language Acquisition 4.2 Phonological Development 4.3 Grammar Development 5 Music Pattern Recognition 5.1 Cognitive Mechanisms 5.2 Developmental Issues 6 False pattern recognition 7 See also 8 Notes 9 References 10 External links


Theories[edit] Template matching[edit] Template matching theory describes the most basic approach to human pattern recognition. It is a theory that assumes every perceived object is stored as a “template” into long-term memory [4]. Then, incoming information is compared to these templates to find an exact match [5]. In other words, all sensory input is compared to multiple representations of an object to form one single conceptual understanding. The theory defines perception as a fundamentally recognition-based process. It assumes that everything we see, we understand only through past exposure, which then informs our future perception of the external world [6]. For example, A A A are all recognized as the letter A, but not B. This viewpoint is limited, however, in explaining how new experiences can be understood without being compared to a internal memory template [7]. Prototype matching[edit] Unlike the exact, one-to-one, template matching theory, prototype matching instead compares incoming sensory input to one average prototype [7]. This theory proposes that exposure to a series of related stimuli leads to the creation of a “typical” prototype based on their shared features [6]. It reduces the number of stored templates by standardizing them into a single representation [4]. The prototype supports perceptual flexibility, because unlike in template matching, it allows for variability in the recognition of novel stimuli [7]. For instance, if a child had never seen a lawn chair before, they would still be able to recognize it as a chair because of their understanding of its essential characteristics as having four legs and a seat. This idea, however, limits the conceptualization of objects that cannot necessarily be “averaged” into one, like types of canines, for instance. Even though dogs, wolves, and foxes are all typically furry, four-legged, moderately sized animals with ears and a tail, they are not all the same, and thus cannot be strictly perceived with respect to the prototype matching theory. Feature analysis[edit] There are multiple theories that try to answer how humans are able to recognize patterns in their environment. Feature Detection Theory proposes that the nervous system sorts and filters incoming stimuli to allow the human (or animal) to make sense of the information. In the organism, this system is made up of Feature Detectors that are individual neurons, or groups of neurons, that encode specific perceptual features. The theory proposes an increasing complexity in the relationship between detectors and the perceptual feature. The most basic level feature detectors respond to simple properties of the stimuli. Further along the perceptual pathway, higher organized feature detectors are able to respond to more complex and specific stimuli properties [8]. When features repeat or occur in a meaningful sequence, we are able to identify these patterns because of our feature detection system. Multiple Discrimination Scaling[edit] Template and feature analysis approaches to recognition of objects (and situations) have been merged / reconciled / overtaken by multiple discrimination theory. This states that the amounts in a test stimulus of each salient feature of a template are recognized in any perceptual judgment as being at a distance in the universal unit of 50% discrimination (the objective performance 'JND': Torgerson, 1958) from the amount of that feature in the template (Booth & Freeman, 1993, Acta Psychologica). Recognition by Components Theory[edit] Image showing the breakdown of common geometric shapes (geons) Similar to Feature Detection Theory, Recognition by Components (RBC) focus on the bottom-up features of the stimuli being processed. First proposed by Irving Biederman (1987) this theory states that humans recognize objects by breaking them down into their basic 3D geometric shapes called geons (i.e. cylinders, cubes, cones, etc.). An example is how we break down a common item like a coffee cup, we recognize the hollow cylinder that holds the liquid and a curved handle off the side that allows us to hold it. Even though not every coffee cup is exactly the same, these basic components helps us to recognize the consistency across examples (or pattern). RBC suggest that are fewer than 36 unique geons that when combined can form a virtually unlimited number of objects. To parse and dissect an object, RBC proposed we attend to two specific features: edges and concavities. Edges enable the observer to maintain a consistent representation of the object regardless of the viewing angle and lighting conditions. Concavities are where two edges meet and enable the observer to perceive where one geon ends and another begins. Interestingly, the RBC principles of visual object recognition can be applied to auditory language recognition as well. In place of geons, language researchers propose that spoken language can be broken down into basic components called phonemes. There are 44 phonemes in the English language and it takes 55 phonemes to encompass the entirety of all spoken human language [9]. Top-down and Bottom-up Processing[edit] Top-Down Processing[edit] Top-down processing refers to the use of background information in pattern recognition [10] and always begins with a person’s previous knowledge, and predictions due to this already acquired knowledge [11] Psychologist Richard Gregory estimated that about 90% of the information is lost between the time it takes to go from the eye to brain and that is why the brain must guess what the person sees based on past experiences. In other words, we construct our perception of reality and these perceptions are hypotheses or propositions based on past experiences and stored information. The formation of incorrect propositions will lead to errors of perception such as visual illusions [10] An example of top down processing is if you are presented with a paragraph written with difficult handwriting. It is easier to understand what the writer wants to convey if you read the whole paragraph rather than reading the words in separate terms. The brain may be able to perceive and understand the gist of the paragraph due to the context supplied by the surrounding words[12] Bottom-up Processing[edit] Bottom-up processing is also known as data-driven processing, because it originates with the stimulation of the sensory receptors [11] . Psychologist James Gibson opposed the top-down model and argued that perception is direct, and not subject to hypotheses testing as Gregory proposed. He stated that sensation is perception and there is no need for extra interpretation as there is enough information in our environment to make sense of the world in a direct way. His theory is sometimes known as the ‘Ecological Theory’ because of the claim that perception can be explained solely in terms of the environment. An example of bottom up processing is that there is a flower at the center of a person's field. The sight of the flower and all the information about the stimulus are carried from the retina to the visual cortex in the brain. The signal travels in one direction [12]


Seriation[edit] A simple seriation task involving arranging shapes by size. In psychologist Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, the third stage is called the Concrete Operational State. It is during this stage that the abstract principle of thinking called 'Seriation' is naturally developed in a child [13] Seriation is the ability to arrange items in a logical order along a quantitative dimension such as length, weight, age, etc. [14] It is a general cognitive skill which is not fully mastered until after the nursery years [15] To seriate means to understand that objects can be ordered along the dimension [13], and to effectively do so, the child needs to be able to answer the question, “What comes next?” [15] Seriation skills also help to develop problem-solving skills which are useful in recognizing and completing patterning tasks. Piaget's Work on Seriation[edit] Piaget studied the development of seriation along with Szeminska in an experiment where they used 10 small rods along with another set of rods of medium length to test the skill of children [16] In this study they found that there were three distinct stages of development of the skill. In the first stage, children who were around the age of 4 could not arrange the first ten rods in order. They could make smaller groups of 2-4 but could not put all the elements together. In the second stage where the children were 5-6 years of age, they could succeed in the seriation task with the first ten rods through the process of trial and error. For the other set of rods, they could insert those into order through trial and error. In the third stage, the 7-8-year-old children could arrange the rods into order, both sets, without much trial and error. The children used the systematic method of first looking for the smallest rod first and the smallest among the rest. [16] Development of Problem Solving Skills[edit] To develop the skill of seriation which then helps advance a child’s problem-solving skills, children should be provided with the opportunities to arrange things in order using the appropriate language, such as big and bigger when working with size relations. In addition to size arrangement, children should also be given the chance to arrange objects in order based on the texture, sound, flavor and color. [15]Along with specific tasks of seriation, children should be given the chance to compare the different materials and toys they use during play. Through activities like these, the true understanding of characteristics of objects will develop. To aid them at a young age, the differences between the objects should be obvious. [15]Lastly, a more complicated task of arranging two different sets of objects and seeing the relationship between the two different sets should also be provided. A common example of this activity is by having children attempt to fit saucepan lids to saucepans of different sizes or fitting together different sizes of nuts and bolts [15] Application of Seriation in Schools[edit] To help build up math skills in children, teachers and parents can help them learn seriation and patterning. Young children who understand seriation can put numbers in order from lowest to highest, smallest to largest. Eventually, they will come to understand that 6 is higher than 5 or 20 is higher than 10. [17] Similarly, having children copy patterns or create patterns of their own, like ABAB patterns is a great way to help children recognize order and prepare for later math skills, such as multiplication. Child care providers can begin exposing children to patterns at a very young age by having them make groups and count the total number of objects. For example, four groups of three objects each make a total of 12 objects (4 X 3 = 12) [17]


Facial Pattern Recognition[edit] Recognizing faces is one of the most common forms of pattern recognition. Humans are incredibly effective at remembering faces but this ease and automaticity belies a very challenging problem [18] [19]. All faces are physically similar to one another. Faces have two eyes, one mouth, and one nose all in predictable locations, yet humans can recognize a face from several different angles and even various lighting conditions [19]. Neuroscientists posit that recognizing faces takes place in three phases. The first phase starts with visually focusing on of the physical features of the stimuli (in this case the face). After focusing on of the facial features takes place, the facial recognition system then needs to reconstruct the identity of the person from previous experiences. This construction provides us with the signal that this might be a person we know. The third and final phase of recognition completes when the face elicits the name associated with the person [20]. Although humans are great at recognizing faces under normal viewing angles, upside down faces are tremendously difficult to recognize. This demonstrates not only the challenges of facial recognition but also how humans have specialized procedures and capacities for recognizing faces under normal upright viewing conditions [19]. Neural Mechanisms[edit] Brain animation highlighting the Fusiform Face Area; thought to be where facial processing and recognition takes place Scientists agree that the there is a certain area in the brain that is specifically devoted to processing faces. This structure is called the fusiform gyrus and brain imaging studies have shown this area becomes highly active when a subject is viewing a face [21]. Additionally, several case studies have reported that patients with lesions or tissue damage localized to this area have tremendous difficulty recognizing faces, even their own. Although most of this research is circumstantial, a recent study at Stanford University provided conclusive evidence for the fusiform gyrus’ role in facial recognition. In a unique case study, researcher were able to send direct signals to a patient’s fusiform gyrus. The patient reported that the faces of the doctors and nurses changed and morphed in front of him during this electrical stimulation. Researchers agree this demonstrates convincing causal link between this neural structure and the human ability to recognize faces [21]. Facial Recognition Development[edit] Although in adults, facial recognition is fast and automatic, children do not reach adult levels of performance (in laboratory tasks) until adolescence [22]. Two general theories have been put forth to explain how facial recognition normally develops. The first theory is called General Cognitive Development Theory proposes that the perceptual ability to encode faces is fully developed early in childhood, and that the continued improvement of facial recognition into adulthood is attributed to other general factors. These general factors include improved attentional focus, deliberate task strategies, and metacognition. Research supports the argument that these other general factors improve dramatically into adulthood [22]. The second theory, called Face-specific Perceptual Development Theory, argues that the improved facial recognition between children and adults is due to a precise development of facial perception. The cause for this continued to develop is proposed to be an ongoing experience with faces. Developmental Issues[edit] Several developmental issues manifest themselves as a decreased capacity for facial recognition. Using what is known about the role of the fusiform gyrus, research has shown that impaired social development along the autism spectrum is accompanied by a behavioral marker where these individuals tend to look away from faces and a neurological marker characterized by decreased neural activity in the fusiform gyrus. Similarly, those with developmental prosopagnosia (DP) struggle with facial recognition to the extent they are often unable to pick out even their own face. Many studies report that around 2% of the world’s population have developmental prosopagnosia and that individuals with DP have a family history of the trait [19]. Interestingly, individual with DP are behaviorally indistinguishable from those with physical damage or lesions on the fusiform gyrus, again implicating it importance to facial recognition. Despite those with DP or neurological damage, there remains a large variability in facial recognition ability in the total population [19]. The question is what accounts for the differences in facial recognition ability, is this a biological or environmental disposition? Recent research analyzing identical and fraternal twins showed that facial recognition was significantly higher correlated in identical twins, suggesting a strong genetic component to individual differences in facial recognition ability [19].


Language Development[edit] Pattern Recognition in Language Acquisition[edit] Recent research reveals that infant language acquisition is linked to cognitive pattern recognition [23]. Unlike classical nativist and behavioral theories of language development [24], scientists now believe that language is a learnt skill [23]. Studies at the Hebrew University and the University of Sydney both show a strong correlation between the ability to identify visual patterns and learn a new language [23][25]. Children with high shape recognition showed better grammar knowledge, even when controlling for the effects of intelligence and memory capacity [25]. This is supported by the theory that language learning is based on statistical learning [23], the process by which infants perceive common combinations of sounds and words in language and them to inform future speech production. Phonological Development[edit] The first step in infant language acquisition is to decipher between the most basic sound units of their native language. This includes every consonant, every short and long vowel sound, and any additional letter combinations like “th” and “ph” in English, for example. These units, called phonemes, are detected through exposure and pattern recognition. Infants use their “innate feature detector” capabilities to distinguish between the sounds of words [24]. They split them into phonemes through a mechanism of categorical perception. Then, they extract statistical information by recognizing which combinations of sounds are most likely to occur together [24], like “qu” or “h” plus a vowel. In this way, their ability to learn words is based directly on the accuracy of their earlier phonetic patterning. Grammar Development[edit] The transition from phonemic differentiation into higher-order word production [24] is only the first step in the hierarchical acquisition of language. Pattern recognition is furthermore utilized in the detection of prosody cues, the stress and intonation patterns among words [24]. Then, it is applied further still to sentence structure and the understanding of typical clause boundaries [24].  This entire process is reflected in reading as well. First, a child recognizes patterns of individual letters, then words, then groups of words together, then paragraphs, and finally, entire chapters in books [26]. Learning to read and learning to speak a language are based on the “stepwise refinement of patterns” [26] in perceptual pattern recognition.


Music Pattern Recognition[edit] Music provides deep and special emotional experiences for the listener [27]. These experiences become contents in the long-term memory, and every time we listen to or hear the same tunes, those contents are activated. Recognizing the content by the pattern of the music affects our emotion. The mechanism that forms the pattern recognition of music and the experience has been studied by multiple researchers. The sensation felt when listening to our favorite music is evident by the dilation of the pupils, the increase in pulse and blood pressure, the streaming of blood to the leg muscles and the activation of the cerebellum – brain region associated with physical movement [27]. While retrieving the memory of a tune demonstrates general recognition of musical pattern, pattern recognition also occurs while listening to a tune for the first time. The recurring nature of the metre allows the listener to follow a tune, recognize the metre, expect its upcoming occurrence and figure the rhythm. The excitement of following a familiar music pattern happens when the pattern breaks and becomes unpredictable. This following and breaking of a pattern creates a problem-solving opportunity for the mind that form the experience [27]. Psychologist, Daniel Levitin argues that the repetitions, melodic nature and organization of this music creates meaning for the brain [28]. The brain stores information in an arrangement of neurons which retrieve the same information when activated by the environment. By constantly referencing information and additional stimulation from the environment the brain constructs musical features into a perceptual whole [28]. The medial prefrontal cortex – one of the last areas affected by Alzheimer’s disease – is the region activated by music. Cognitive Mechanisms[edit] To understand music pattern recognition, we need to understand the underlying cognitive systems that each handle a part of this process. Various activities are at work in this recognition of a piece of music and its patterns. Researchers have begun to unveil the reasons behind the stimulated reactions to music. Montreal based researchers asked ten volunteers who got chills listening to music to bring their favorite songs and listen to the tracks while their brain activity was being monitored [27]. The results show the significant role of the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) region – involved with cognitive processes such as motivation, reward, addiction, etc. – creating the neural arrangements that make up the experience [27]. A sense of reward prediction is created by anticipation before the climax of the tune, that comes to a sense of resolution when the climax is reached. The longer the listener is denied the expected pattern, the greater the emotional arousal when the pattern returns. Musicologist Leonard Meyer used fifty measures of Beethoven’s 5th movement of the String Quartet in C-sharp minor, Op. 131 to examine this notion [27]. The stronger this experience is, the more vivid memory it will create and store. This strength affects the speed and accuracy of retrieval and recognition of the musical pattern. The brain not only recognizes specific tunes, it distinguishes standard acoustic features, speech and music. MIT researchers conducted a study to examine this notion [29]. The results showed six neural clusters in the auditory cortex responding to the sounds. Four of them triggered when hearing standard acoustic features, one specifically responded to speech and the last exclusively responded to music. Researchers who studied the correlation between temporal evolution of timbral, tonal and rhythmic features of music, came to the conclusion that music engages the brain regions connected to motor actions, emotions and creativity. The research indicates that the whole brain lights up when listening to music [30]. This amount of activity boosts memory preservation, hence pattern recognition. Recognizing patterns of music is different for a musician and a listener. Although a musician may play the same notes every time, the details of the frequency will always be different. The listener however, will recognize the musical pattern and their types despite the variations. These musical types are conceptual and learned, meaning they might vary culturally [31]. While listeners are involved with recognizing (implicit) musical material, musicians are involved with recalling (explicit) them [2]. A UCLA study found that when watching or hearing music being played, neurons associated with the muscles needed for playing the instrument fire. Mirror neurons light up when musicians and non-musicians listen to a piece [32]. Developmental Issues[edit] Pattern recognition of music can build and strengthen other skills; such as, musical synchrony and attentional performance and musical notation and brain engagement. Even a couple of years of musical training, enhances memory and attentions levels. Scientists at University of Newcastle, conducted a study on patients with severe acquired brain injuries (ABIs) and healthy participants, using popular music to examine music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs) [30]. All of the participants were asked to record their familiarity with the song, whether they liked them and what memories they evoked. The results showed that the ABI patients had the highest MEAMs and all the participants had MEAMs of a person, people or a life period that were generally positive [30]. The participants completed the task by utilizing pattern recognition skills. Memory evocation caused the songs to sound more familiar and well liked. This research can be beneficial to rehabilitating patients of autobiographical amnesia who did not have fundamental deficiency in autobiographical recall memory and intact pitch perception [30]. In a study at University of California, Davis mapped the brain of participants while they listened to music [33]. The results showed links between brain regions to autobiographical memories and emotions activated by familiar music. This study can explain the strong response of patients with Alzheimer’s disease to music. This research can help such patients with pattern recognition enhancing tasks.


False pattern recognition[edit] Main article: Apophenia Whale, submarine or sheep? The human tendency to see patterns that do not actually exist is called apophenia. Examples of apophenia include the Man in the Moon, faces or figures in shadows, in clouds, and in patterns with no deliberate design, such as the swirls on a baked confection, and the perception of causal relationships between events which are, in fact, unrelated. Apophenia figures prominently in conspiracy theories, gambling, misinterpretation of statistics and scientific data, and some kinds of religious and paranormal experiences. Misperception of patterns in random data is called pareidolia.


See also[edit] Gambler's fallacy Gestalt psychology Pareidolia Thin-slicing


Notes[edit]


References[edit] ^ Eysenck, Michael W.; Keane, Mark T. (2003). Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook (4th ed.). Hove; Philadelphia; New York: Taylor & Francis. ISBN 9780863775512. OCLC 894210185. Retrieved 27 November 2014. ^ a b Snyder, B. (2000). Music and memory: An introduction. MIT press. ^ Mattson, M. P. (2014). Superior pattern processing is the essence of the evolved human brain. Frontiers in neuroscience, 8. ^ a b Shugen, W. (2002). Framework of pattern recognition model based on the cognitive psychology. Geo-spatial Information Science, 5(2), 74-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02833890 ^ H. Gregg. (2013, May 1). Perception and perceptual illusions. Psychology Today. Retrieved from https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/theory-knowledge/201305/perception-and-perceptual-illusions ^ a b Top-down and bottom-up theories of perception. (2009, June 27). Retrieved from http://cognitivepsychology.wikidot.com/cognition:topdown ^ a b c Y. Pi, W. Liao, M. Liu, & J. Lu. (2008). Theory of cognitive pattern recognition. INTECH. Retrieved from http://cdn.intechopen.com/pdfs/5795.pdf ^ Feature Detection (nervous system):https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Feature_detection_(nervous_system) ^ Recognition by components theory: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Recognition-by-components_theory ^ a b McLeod, S. (2008) Visual Perception Theory. Simply Psychology. Retrieved from https://www.simplypsychology.org/perception-theories.html ^ a b Wede, J. (2014, April 28). Bottom-up and Top-down Processing: A Collaborative Duality. Retrieved from: http://sites.psu.edu/psych256sp14/2014/04/28/bottom-up-and-top-down-processing-a-collaborative-duality/ ^ a b Sincero, S. M. (2013) Top-Down VS Bottom-Up Processing. Retrieved Oct 20, 2017 from Explorable.com: https://explorable.com/top-down-vs-bottom-up-processing ^ a b Kidd, J. K., Curby, T. W., Boyer, C., Gadzichowski, K., Gallington, D. A., Machado, J. A., & Pasnak, R. (2012). Benefits of an intervention focused on oddity and seriation. Early Education and Development, 23(6), 900-918 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2011.621877 ^ Berk, L. E. (2013). Development through the lifespan (6th ed.). Pearson. ISBN 9780205957606 ^ a b c d e Curtis, A. (2002). Curriculum for the pre-school child. Routledge. ISBN 9781134770458 ^ a b Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1964). Early growth of logic in the child; classification and seriation, aby Bärbel Inhelder and Jean Piaget. New York: Routledge and Paul. ^ a b Basic Math Skills in Child Care: Creating Patterns and Arranging Objects in Order. Retrieved from Extension Articles on 2017-10-20 http://articles.extension.org/pages/25597/basic-math-skills-in-child-care:-creating-patterns-and-arranging-objects-in-order ^ Sheikh, K. (2017) How We Save Face--Researchers Crack the Brain's Facial-Recognition Code. Scientific American. Retrieved from: https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/how-we-save-face-researchers-crack-the-brains-facial-recognition-code/ ^ a b c d e f Duchaine, B. (2015).Individual differences in face recognition ability: Impacts on law enforcement, criminal justice and national security. APA: Psychological Science Agenda. Retrieved from: http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2015/06/face-recognition.aspx ^ Wlassoff, V. (2015). How the Brain Recognizes Faces. Brain Blogger. Retrieved from: http://brainblogger.com/2015/10/17/how-the-brain-recognizes-faces/ ^ a b Norton, E. (2012). Identifying the Brain's own Facial Recognition System. Science Magazine. Retrieved from: http://www.sciencemag.org/news/2012/10/identifying-brains-own-facial-recognition-system ^ a b McKone, E., et al. (2012). A critical review of the development of face recognition: Experience is less important than previously believed. Cognitive Neuropsychology. doi 10.1080/02643294.2012.660138 ^ a b c d Language ability linked to pattern recognition. (2013, May 29). VOA. Retrieved October 25, 2017 from https://www.voanews.com/a/language-ability-linked-to-pattern-recognition/1670776.html ^ a b c d e f Kuhl, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(22), 11850–11857. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.22.11850 ^ a b University of Sydney. (2016, May 5). Pattern learning key to children's language development. ScienceDaily. Retrieved October 25, 2017 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2016/05/160505222938.htm ^ a b Basulto, D. (2013, July 24). Humans are the world’s best pattern-recognition machines, but for how long? Retrieved October 25, 2017 from http://bigthink.com/endless-innovation/humans-are-the-worlds-best-pattern-recognition-machines-but-for-how-long ^ a b c d e f Lehrer, Jonah. “The Neuroscience Of Music.” Wired, Conde Nast, 3 June 2017, www.wired.com/2011/01/the-neuroscience-of-music/. ^ a b Levitin, D. J. (2006). This is your brain on music: The science of a human obsession. Penguin. ^ Bushak, L. (2017). This Is Your Brain On Music: How Our Brains Process Melodies That Pull On Our Heartstrings. [online] Medical Daily. Available at: http://www.medicaldaily.com/your-brain-music-how-our-brains-process-melodies-pull-our-heartstrings-271007 [Accessed 26 Oct. 2017] ^ a b c d Bergland, C. (2013, December 11). Why Do the Songs from Your Past Evoke Such Vivid Memories? Retrieved from https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-athletes-way/201312/why-do-the-songs-your-past-evoke-such-vivid-memories ^ Agus, T. R., Thorpe, S. J., & Pressnitzer, D. (2010). Rapid formation of robust auditory memories: insights from noise. Neuron, 66(4), 610-618. ^ Byrne, D. (2012, October). How Do Our Brains Process Music? Retrieved from https://www.smithsonianmag.com/arts-culture/how-do-our-brains-process-music-32150302/?no-ist=&page=1 ^ Greensfelder, L. (2009, February). Study Finds Brain Hub that Links Music, Memory and Emotion. Retrieved from https://www.ucdavis.edu/news/study-finds-brain-hub-links-music-memory-and-emotion


External links[edit] Media related to Visual pattern recognition at Wikimedia Commons v t e Hidden messages Main Hidden messages Subliminal message Audio Backmasking Reverse speech Numeric Numerology Theomatics Bible code Cryptology Visual Fnord Paranoiac-critical method Pareidolia Sacred geometry Steganography Visual cryptography Other Anagram Apophenia Asemic writing Easter egg Clustering illusion Observer-expectancy effect Pattern recognition Palindrome Unconscious mind v t e Patterns in nature Patterns Crack Dune Foam Meander Phyllotaxis Soap bubble Symmetry in crystals Quasicrystals in flowers in biology Tessellation Vortex street Wave Widmanstätten pattern Causes Pattern formation Biology Natural selection Camouflage Mimicry Sexual selection Mathematics Chaos theory Fractal Logarithmic spiral Physics Crystal Fluid dynamics Plateau's laws Self-organization People Plato Pythagoras Empedocles Leonardo Fibonacci Liber Abaci Adolf Zeising Ernst Haeckel Joseph Plateau Wilson Bentley D'Arcy Wentworth Thompson On Growth and Form Alan Turing The Chemical Basis of Morphogenesis Aristid Lindenmayer Benoît Mandelbrot How Long Is the Coast of Britain? Statistical Self-Similarity and Fractional Dimension Related Pattern recognition Emergence Mathematics and art Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Pattern_recognition_(psychology)&oldid=814263947" Categories: PerceptionCognitionCognitive psychologyPattern recognitionHidden categories: Articles containing video clips


Navigation menu Personal tools Not logged inTalkContributionsCreate accountLog in Namespaces ArticleTalk Variants Views ReadEditView history More Search Navigation Main pageContentsFeatured contentCurrent eventsRandom articleDonate to WikipediaWikipedia store Interaction HelpAbout WikipediaCommunity portalRecent changesContact page Tools What links hereRelated changesUpload fileSpecial pagesPermanent linkPage informationWikidata itemCite this page Print/export Create a bookDownload as PDFPrintable version In other projects Wikimedia Commons Languages Add links This page was last edited on 7 December 2017, at 19:37. Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization. Privacy policy About Wikipedia Disclaimers Contact Wikipedia Developers Cookie statement Mobile view (window.RLQ=window.RLQ||[]).push(function(){mw.config.set({"wgPageParseReport":{"limitreport":{"cputime":"0.184","walltime":"0.233","ppvisitednodes":{"value":1752,"limit":1000000},"ppgeneratednodes":{"value":0,"limit":1500000},"postexpandincludesize":{"value":24426,"limit":2097152},"templateargumentsize":{"value":1681,"limit":2097152},"expansiondepth":{"value":16,"limit":40},"expensivefunctioncount":{"value":0,"limit":500},"entityaccesscount":{"value":1,"limit":400},"timingprofile":["100.00% 171.168 1 -total"," 35.19% 60.242 2 Template:Reflist"," 17.93% 30.682 3 Template:ISBN"," 13.72% 23.490 1 Template:Commons_category-inline"," 13.21% 22.609 1 Template:Other_uses"," 12.58% 21.530 1 Template:Sister-inline"," 7.68% 13.143 3 Template:Catalog_lookup_link"," 7.36% 12.598 2 Template:Navbox"," 6.25% 10.704 1 Template:Hidden_messages"," 5.02% 8.595 1 Template:Notelist"]},"scribunto":{"limitreport-timeusage":{"value":"0.020","limit":"10.000"},"limitreport-memusage":{"value":1538277,"limit":52428800}},"cachereport":{"origin":"mw1325","timestamp":"20180107053352","ttl":1900800,"transientcontent":false}}});});(window.RLQ=window.RLQ||[]).push(function(){mw.config.set({"wgBackendResponseTime":77,"wgHostname":"mw1215"});});


Pattern_recognition_(psychology) - Photos and All Basic Informations

Pattern_recognition_(psychology) More Links

Pattern Recognition (disambiguation)PsychologyCognitive NeuroscienceCognitiveStimulus (psychology)Recall (memory)MemoryShort-term MemoryLong-term MemorySemantic MemoryNeural NetworksPrototype-matchingRecognition-by-components TheoryFourier AnalysisLanguageMusicEnlargeEnlargeJean PiagetCognitive DevelopmentProblem-solvingEnlargeFusiform GyrusAutism SpectrumFusiform GyrusProsopagnosiaProsopagnosiaFusiform GyrusTwinsTwinsLanguage AcquisitionPsychological NativismBehaviorismLanguage DevelopmentIntelligenceMemoryStatistical Learning In Language AcquisitionPhonemesFeature Detection (nervous System)Categorical PerceptionProsody (linguistics)ClauseMusicLong-term MemoryCerebellumMemoryMetreMetreRhythmProblem-solvingNeuronsPrefrontal CortexAlzheimer’s DiseaseNucleus AccumbensBeethovenAuditory CortexMirror NeuronsAcquired Brain InjuriesAutobiographical MemoriesAutobiographical MemoriesAlzheimer’s DiseaseApopheniaEnlargeWhaleSubmarineSheepApopheniaSynchronicityConspiracy TheoriesGamblingStatisticsScientificReligionParanormalPareidoliaGambler's FallacyGestalt PsychologyPareidoliaThin-slicingInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/9780863775512International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/9780205957606International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/9781134770458Commons:Category:Visual Pattern RecognitionTemplate:Hidden MessagesTemplate Talk:Hidden MessagesHidden MessageHidden MessageSubliminal StimuliBackmaskingReverse SpeechNumerologyTheomaticsBible CodeCryptographyFnordParanoiac-critical MethodPareidoliaSacred GeometrySteganographyVisual CryptographyAnagramApopheniaAsemic WritingEaster Egg (media)Clustering IllusionObserver-expectancy EffectPalindromeUnconscious MindTemplate:Patterns In NaturePatterns In NatureFractureDuneFoamMeanderPhyllotaxisSoap BubbleSymmetryCrystal SymmetryQuasicrystalsFloral SymmetrySymmetry In BiologyTessellationVortex StreetWaveWidmanstätten PatternPattern FormationBiologyNatural SelectionCamouflageMimicrySexual SelectionMathematicsChaos TheoryFractalLogarithmic SpiralPhysicsCrystalFluid DynamicsPlateau's LawsSelf-organizationPlatoPythagorasEmpedoclesLeonardo FibonacciLiber AbaciAdolf ZeisingErnst HaeckelJoseph PlateauWilson BentleyD'Arcy Wentworth ThompsonOn Growth And FormAlan TuringThe Chemical Basis Of MorphogenesisAristid LindenmayerBenoit MandelbrotHow Long Is The Coast Of Britain? Statistical Self-Similarity And Fractional DimensionEmergenceMathematics And ArtHelp:CategoryCategory:PerceptionCategory:CognitionCategory:Cognitive PsychologyCategory:Pattern RecognitionCategory:Articles Containing Video ClipsDiscussion About Edits From This IP Address [n]A List Of Edits Made From This IP Address [y]View The Content Page [c]Discussion About The Content Page [t]Edit This Page [e]Visit The Main Page [z]Guides To Browsing WikipediaFeatured Content – The Best Of WikipediaFind Background Information On Current EventsLoad A Random Article [x]Guidance On How To Use And Edit WikipediaFind Out About WikipediaAbout The Project, What You Can Do, Where To Find ThingsA List Of Recent Changes In The Wiki [r]List Of All English Wikipedia Pages Containing Links To This Page [j]Recent Changes In Pages Linked From This Page [k]Upload Files [u]A List Of All Special Pages [q]Wikipedia:AboutWikipedia:General Disclaimer



view link view link view link view link view link