Contents 1 Versions 1.1 Radical behaviorism 1.2 Experimental and conceptual innovations 1.3 Relation to language 2 Education 3 Operant conditioning 4 Classical conditioning 5 Molecular versus molar behaviorism 6 In philosophy 7 21st-century behavior analysis 8 Behavior analysis and culture 9 Behavior informatics and behavior computing 10 Criticisms and limitations of behaviorism 11 List of notable behaviorists 12 See also 12.1 Related therapies 13 References 14 Further reading 15 External links


Versions[edit] There is no universally agreed-upon classification, but some titles given to the various branches of behaviorism include: Methodological behaviorism: Watson's behaviorism states that only public events (behaviors of an individual) can be objectively observed, and that therefore private events (thoughts and feelings) should be ignored.[1][6] It also became the basis for the early approach behavior modification in the late 1970s and early 1980s. Radical behaviorism: Skinner's behaviorism theorizes that processes within the organism should be acknowledged, particularly the presence of private events (such as thoughts and feelings), and suggests that environmental variables also control these internal events just as they control observable behaviors. Radical behaviorism forms the core philosophy behind behavior analysis. Willard Van Orman Quine used many of radical behaviorism's ideas in his study of knowledge and language.[6] Teleological behaviorism: Post-Skinnerian, purposive, close to microeconomics. Focuses on objective observation as opposed to cognitive processes. Theoretical behaviorism: Post-Skinnerian, accepts observable internal states ("within the skin" once meant "unobservable", but with modern technology we are not so constrained); dynamic, but eclectic in choice of theoretical structures, emphasizes parsimony. Biological behaviorism: Post-Skinnerian, centered on perceptual and motor modules of behavior, theory of behavior systems. Psychological behaviorism: As proposed by Arthur W. Staats, this version of behaviorism centers on the practical control of human behavior. It is noted for its use of time-outs, token-reinforcement and other methods, which importantly influenced modern approaches to child development, education, and abnormal psychology.[7][8] Two subtypes are: Hullian and post-Hullian: theoretical, group data, not dynamic, physiological Purposive: Tolman's behavioristic anticipation of cognitive psychology Radical behaviorism[edit] Main article: Radical behaviorism B. F. Skinner proposed radical behaviorism as the conceptual underpinning of the experimental analysis of behavior. This view differs from other approaches to behavioral research in various ways but, most notably here, it contrasts with methodological behaviorism in accepting feelings, states of mind and introspection as behaviors subject to scientific investigation. Like methodological behaviorism it rejects the reflex as a model of all behavior, and it defends the science of behavior as complementary to but independent of physiology. Radical behaviorism overlaps considerably with other western philosophical positions such as American pragmatism.[9] Experimental and conceptual innovations[edit] This essentially philosophical position gained strength from the success of Skinner's early experimental work with rats and pigeons, summarized in his books The Behavior of Organisms[10] and Schedules of Reinforcement.[11] Of particular importance was his concept of the operant response, of which the canonical example was the rat's lever-press. In contrast with the idea of a physiological or reflex response, an operant is a class of structurally distinct but functionally equivalent responses. For example, while a rat might press a lever with its left paw or its right paw or its tail, all of these responses operate on the world in the same way and have a common consequence. Operants are often thought of as species of responses, where the individuals differ but the class coheres in its function-shared consequences with operants and reproductive success with species. This is a clear distinction between Skinner's theory and S–R theory. Skinner's empirical work expanded on earlier research on trial-and-error learning by researchers such as Thorndike and Guthrie with both conceptual reformulations—Thorndike's notion of a stimulus–response "association" or "connection" was abandoned; and methodological ones—the use of the "free operant", so called because the animal was now permitted to respond at its own rate rather than in a series of trials determined by the experimenter procedures. With this method, Skinner carried out substantial experimental work on the effects of different schedules and rates of reinforcement on the rates of operant responses made by rats and pigeons. He achieved remarkable success in training animals to perform unexpected responses, to emit large numbers of responses, and to demonstrate many empirical regularities at the purely behavioral level. This lent some credibility to his conceptual analysis. It is largely his conceptual analysis that made his work much more rigorous than his peers', a point which can be seen clearly in his seminal work Are Theories of Learning Necessary? in which he criticizes what he viewed to be theoretical weaknesses then common in the study of psychology. An important descendant of the experimental analysis of behavior is the Society for Quantitative Analysis of Behavior.[12][13] Relation to language[edit] As Skinner turned from experimental work to concentrate on the philosophical underpinnings of a science of behavior, his attention turned to human language with his 1957 book Verbal Behavior[14] and other language-related publications;[15] Verbal Behavior laid out a vocabulary and theory for functional analysis of verbal behavior, and was strongly criticized in a review by Noam Chomsky.[16][17] Skinner did not respond in detail but claimed that Chomsky failed to understand his ideas,[18] and the disagreements between the two and the theories involved have been further discussed.[19][20] Innateness theory is opposed to behaviorist theory which claims that language is a set of habits that can be acquired by means of conditioning.[21][22] According to some, this process that the behaviorists define is a very slow and gentle process to explain a phenomenon as complicated as language learning. What was important for a behaviorist's analysis of human behavior was not language acquisition so much as the interaction between language and overt behavior. In an essay republished in his 1969 book Contingencies of Reinforcement,[23] Skinner took the view that humans could construct linguistic stimuli that would then acquire control over their behavior in the same way that external stimuli could. The possibility of such "instructional control" over behavior meant that contingencies of reinforcement would not always produce the same effects on human behavior as they reliably do in other animals. The focus of a radical behaviorist analysis of human behavior therefore shifted to an attempt to understand the interaction between instructional control and contingency control, and also to understand the behavioral processes that determine what instructions are constructed and what control they acquire over behavior. Recently, a new line of behavioral research on language was started under the name of relational frame theory.


Education[edit] Behaviourism focuses on one particular view of learning: a change in external behaviour achieved through using reinforcement and repetition (Rote learning) to shape behavior of learners. Skinner found that behaviors could be shaped when the use of reinforcement was implemented. Desired behavior is rewarded, while the undesired behavior is punished.[24] Incorporating behaviorism into the classroom allowed educators to assist their students in excelling both academically and personally. In the field of language learning, this type of teaching was called the audio-lingual method, characterised by the whole class using choral chanting of key phrases, dialogues and immediate correction. Within the behaviourist view of learning, the "teacher" is the dominant person in the classroom and takes complete control, evaluation of learning comes from the teacher who decides what is right or wrong. The learner does not have any opportunity for evaluation or reflection within the learning process, they are simply told what is right or wrong. The conceptualization of learning using this approach could be considered "superficial" as the focus is on external changes in behaviour i.e. not interested in the internal processes of learning leading to behaviour change and has no place for the emotions involved the process.


Operant conditioning[edit] Main article: Operant conditioning Operant conditioning was developed by B.F. Skinner in 1937 and deals with the modification of "voluntary behaviour" or operant behaviour. Operant behavior operates on the environment and is maintained by its consequences. Reinforcement and punishment, the core tools of operant conditioning, are either positive (delivered following a response), or negative (withdrawn following a response).[25] Skinner created the Skinner Box or operant conditioning chamber to test the effects of operant conditioning principles on rats. From this study, he discovered that the rats learned very effectively if they were rewarded frequently. Skinner also found that he could shape the rats' behavior through the use of rewards, which could, in turn, be applied to human learning as well.


Classical conditioning[edit] Main article: Classical conditioning Although operant conditioning plays the largest role in discussions of behavioral mechanisms, classical conditioning (or Pavlovian conditioning or respondent conditioning) is also an important behavior-analytic process that need not refer to mental or other internal processes. Pavlov's experiments with dogs provide the most familiar example of the classical conditioning procedure. In simple conditioning, the dog was presented with a stimulus such as a light or a sound, and then food was placed in the dog's mouth. After a few repetitions of this sequence, the light or sound by itself caused the dog to salivate.[26] Although Pavlov proposed some tentative physiological processes that might be involved in classical conditioning, these have not been confirmed.[citation needed] The idea of classical conditioning helped behaviorist John Watson discover the key mechanism behind how humans acquire the behaviors that they do, which was to find a natural reflex that produces the response being considered. Watson's "Behaviourist Manifesto" has three aspects that deserve special recognition: one is that psychology should be purely objective, with any interpretation of conscious experience being removed, thus leading to psychology as the "science of behaviour"; the second one is that the goals of psychology should be to predict and control behaviour (as opposed to describe and explain conscious mental states; the third one is that there is no notable distinction between human and non-human behaviour. Following Darwin's theory of evolution, this would simply mean that human behaviour is just a more complex version in respect to behaviour displayed by other species.[27]


Molecular versus molar behaviorism[edit] Skinner's view of behavior is most often characterized as a "molecular" view of behavior; that is, behavior can be decomposed into atomistic parts or molecules. This view is inconsistent with Skinner's complete description of behavior as delineated in other works, including his 1981 article "Selection by Consequences".[28] Skinner proposed that a complete account of behavior requires understanding of selection history at three levels: biology (the natural selection or phylogeny of the animal); behavior (the reinforcement history or ontogeny of the behavioral repertoire of the animal); and for some species, culture (the cultural practices of the social group to which the animal belongs). This whole organism then interacts with its environment. Molecular behaviorists use notions from melioration theory, negative power function discounting or additive versions of negative power function discounting.[29] Molar behaviorists, such as Howard Rachlin, Richard Herrnstein, and William Baum, argue that behavior cannot be understood by focusing on events in the moment. That is, they argue that behavior is best understood as the ultimate product of an organism's history and that molecular behaviorists are committing a fallacy by inventing fictitious proximal causes for behavior. Molar behaviorists argue that standard molecular constructs, such as "associative strength", are better replaced by molar variables such as rate of reinforcement.[30] Thus, a molar behaviorist would describe "loving someone" as a pattern of loving behavior over time; there is no isolated, proximal cause of loving behavior, only a history of behaviors (of which the current behavior might be an example) that can be summarized as "love".


In philosophy[edit] Behaviorism is a psychological movement that can be contrasted with philosophy of mind. The basic premise of radical behaviorism is that the study of behavior should be a natural science, such as chemistry or physics, without any reference to hypothetical inner states of organisms as causes for their behavior. Less radical varieties are unconcerned with philosophical positions on internal, mental and subjective experience. Behaviorism takes a functional view of behavior. According to Edmund Fantino and colleagues: "Behavior analysis has much to offer the study of phenomena normally dominated by cognitive and social psychologists. We hope that successful application of behavioral theory and methodology will not only shed light on central problems in judgment and choice but will also generate greater appreciation of the behavioral approach."[31] Behaviorist sentiments are not uncommon within philosophy of language and analytic philosophy. It is sometimes argued that Ludwig Wittgenstein defended a behaviorist position (e.g., the beetle in a box argument)—but while there are important relations between his thought and behaviorism, the claim that he was a behaviorist is quite controversial. Mathematician Alan Turing is also sometimes considered a behaviorist,[citation needed] but he himself did not make this identification. In logical and empirical positivism (as held, e.g., by Rudolf Carnap and Carl Hempel), the meaning of psychological statements are their verification conditions, which consist of performed overt behavior. W.V. Quine made use of a type of behaviorism, influenced by some of Skinner's ideas, in his own work on language. Gilbert Ryle defended a distinct strain of philosophical behaviorism, sketched in his book The Concept of Mind. Ryle's central claim was that instances of dualism frequently represented "category mistakes", and hence that they were really misunderstandings of the use of ordinary language. Daniel Dennett likewise acknowledges himself to be a type of behaviorist,[32] though he offers extensive criticism of radical behaviorism and refutes Skinner's rejection of the value of intentional idioms and the possibility of free will.[33] This is Dennett's main point in "Skinner Skinned." Dennett argues that there is a crucial difference between explaining and explaining away… If our explanation of apparently rational behavior turns out to be extremely simple, we may want to say that the behavior was not really rational after all. But if the explanation is very complex and intricate, we may want to say not that the behavior is not rational, but that we now have a better understanding of what rationality consists in. (Compare: if we find out how a computer program solves problems in linear algebra, we don't say it's not really solving them, we just say we know how it does it. On the other hand, in cases like Weizenbaum's ELIZA program, the explanation of how the computer carries on a conversation is so simple that the right thing to say seems to be that the machine isn't really carrying on a conversation, it's just a trick.) — Curtis Brown, Philosophy of Mind, "Behaviorism: Skinner and Dennett"[34]


21st-century behavior analysis[edit] This section contains content that is written like an advertisement. Please help improve it by removing promotional content and inappropriate external links, and by adding encyclopedic content written from a neutral point of view. (March 2015) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) The early term behavior modification has been obsolete since the 1990s as it currently refers to the brief revival of methodological behaviorism in the late 1950s and again from the late 1970s to early 1980s.[35][36][37] Applied behavior analysis—the term that replaced behavior modification—has emerged into a thriving field. The Association for Behavior Analysis: International (ABAI) currently has 32 state and regional chapters within the United States. Approximately 30 additional chapters have also developed throughout Europe, Asia, South America, and the South Pacific. In addition to 34 annual conferences held by ABAI in the United States and Canada, ABAI held the 5th annual International conference in Norway in 2009. The independent development of behaviour analysis outside the US also continues to develop. For example, the UK Society for Behaviour Analysis[38] was founded in 2013 to further the advancement of the science and practice of behaviour analysis across the UK. And in terms of motivation, there remains strong interest in the variety of human motivational behaviour factors, e.g.,[39][40][41][42][43] indeed one could argue that the entire career counselling and advisory industry has at least partly been predicated on analysing individual behaviours.[44] Some, may go as far as suggesting that the current rapid change in organisational behaviour could partly be attributed to some of these theories and the theories that are related to it.[45] The interests among behavior analysts today are wide-ranging, as a review of the 30 Special Interest Groups (SIGs) within ABAI indicates. Such interests include everything from developmental disabilities and autism, to cultural psychology, clinical psychology, verbal behavior, Organizational Behavior Management (OBM; behavior analytic I–O psychology). OBM has developed a particularly strong following within behavior analysis, as evidenced by the formation of the OBM Network and the influential Journal of Organizational Behavior Management (JOBM; recently rated the 3rd highest impact journal in applied psychology by ISI JOBM rating). Applications of behavioral technology, also known as applied behavior analysis or ABA, have been particularly well established in the area of developmental disabilities since the 1960s. Treatment of individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders has grown especially rapidly since the mid-1990s. This demand for services encouraged the formation of a professional credentialing program administered by the Behavior Analyst Certification Board, Inc. (BACB) and accredited by the National Commission for Certifying Agencies. As of early 2012, there are over 300 BACB approved course sequences offered by about 200 colleges and universities worldwide preparing students for this credential and approximately 11,000 BACB certificants, most working in the United States. The Association of Professional Behavior Analysts was formed in 2008 to meet the needs of these ABA professionals. Modern behavior analysis has also witnessed a massive resurgence in research and applications related to language and cognition, with the development of relational frame theory (RFT; described as a "Post-Skinnerian account of language and cognition").[46] RFT also forms the empirical basis for the highly successful and data-driven acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). In fact, researchers and practitioners in RFT/ACT have become sufficiently prominent that they have formed their own specialized organization that is highly behaviorally oriented, known as the Association for Contextual Behavioral Science (ACBS). It has rapidly grown in its few years of existence to reach about 5,000 members worldwide. Some of the current prominent behavior analytic journals include the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (JABA), the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior (JEAB) JEAB website, the Journal of Organizational Behavior Management (JOBM), Behavior and Social Issues (BSI), as well as the Psychological Record. Currently, the US has 14 ABAI accredited MA and PhD programs for comprehensive study in behavior analysis.


Behavior analysis and culture[edit] Cultural analysis has always been at the philosophical core of radical behaviorism from the early days (as seen in Skinner's Walden Two, Science & Human Behavior, Beyond Freedom & Dignity, and About Behaviorism). During the 1980s, behavior analysts, most notably Sigrid Glenn, had a productive interchange with cultural anthropologist Marvin Harris (the most notable proponent of "cultural materialism") regarding interdisciplinary work. Very recently, behavior analysts have produced a set of basic exploratory experiments in an effort toward this end.[47] Behaviorism is also frequently used in game development, although this application is controversial.[48]


Behavior informatics and behavior computing[edit] With the fast growth of big behavioral data and applications, behavior analysis is ubiquitous. Understanding behavior from the informatics and computing perspective becomes increasingly critical for in-depth understanding of what, why and how behaviors are formed, interact, evolve, change and affect business and decision. Behavior informatics[49][50] and behavior computing[51][52] deeply explore behavior intelligence and behavior insights from the informatics and computing perspectives.


Criticisms and limitations of behaviorism[edit] See also: Cognitive psychology and Cognitive neuroscience This section includes a list of references, related reading or external links, but its sources remain unclear because it lacks inline citations. Please help to improve this section by introducing more precise citations. (June 2016) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) In the second half of the 20th century, behaviorism was largely eclipsed as a result of the cognitive revolution.[53][54] This shift was due to methodological behaviorism being highly criticized for not examining mental processes, and this led to the development of the cognitive therapy movement. In the mid-20th century, three main influences arose that would inspire and shape cognitive psychology as a formal school of thought: Noam Chomsky's 1959 critique of behaviorism, and empiricism more generally, initiated what would come to be known as the "cognitive revolution".[55] Developments in computer science would lead to parallels being drawn between human thought and the computational functionality of computers, opening entirely new areas of psychological thought. Allen Newell and Herbert Simon spent years developing the concept of artificial intelligence (AI) and later worked with cognitive psychologists regarding the implications of AI. The effective result was more of a framework conceptualization of mental functions with their counterparts in computers (memory, storage, retrieval, etc.) Formal recognition of the field involved the establishment of research institutions such as George Mandler's Center for Human Information Processing in 1964. Mandler described the origins of cognitive psychology in a 2002 article in the Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences In the early years of cognitive psychology, behaviorist critics held that the empiricism it pursued was incompatible with the concept of internal mental states. Cognitive neuroscience, however, continues to gather evidence of direct correlations between physiological brain activity and putative mental states, endorsing the basis for cognitive psychology.


List of notable behaviorists[edit] Donald Baer Albert Bandura Dermot Barnes-Holmes Vladimir Bekhterev Sidney W. Bijou Jacque Fresco Edwin Ray Guthrie Steven C. Hayes Richard J. Herrnstein Clark L. Hull Brian Iwata Alan E. Kazdin Fred S. Keller Jon Levy Marsha M. Linehan Ole Ivar Lovaas Neal E. Miller O. Hobart Mowrer Charles E. Osgood Ivan Pavlov Murray Sidman B. F. Skinner Kenneth W. Spence J. E. R. Staddon Edward Thorndike Edward C. Tolman John B. Watson Montrose Wolf


See also[edit] Animal training Antecedent stimuli Behavior analysis of child development Behavioral change theories Behavioral economics Behavioral medicine Behavioral neuroscience Counterconditioning Criminology Direct instruction Dog behaviorist Emergency psychiatry Ethology Functional analysis (psychology) List of publications in psychology § Behaviorism The Logic of Modern Physics Mentalism (psychology) Models of abnormality § Behavioural model Observational learning Operationalization Organizational behavior management Pharmacology § Behavioral pharmacology Perceptual control theory Positive behavior support Professional practice of behavior analysis Social skills training Token economy ZebraBox Zoosemiotics Related therapies[edit] Behavior therapy Behavioral activation Clinical behavior analysis Contingency management Dialectical behavior therapy Discrete trial training Exposure and response prevention Exposure therapy Functional analytic psychotherapy Habit reversal training Pivotal response treatment Rational emotive behavior therapy Systematic desensitization


References[edit] ^ a b Chiesa, Mecca (1994). Radical Behaviorism: The Philosophy and the Science. Authors Cooperative, Inc. pp. 1–241. ISBN 0962331147. Retrieved July 31, 2016.  ^ Dillenburger, Karola & Keenan, Mickey (2009). "None of the As in ABA stand for autism: Dispelling the myths". Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability. 34 (2): 193–195. doi:10.1080/13668250902845244. PMID 19404840. Retrieved 2014-12-24.  ^ Baer, Donald M.; Wolf, Montrose M.; Risley, Todd R. (1968). "Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis". Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1 (1): 91–7. doi:10.1901/jaba.1968.1-91. PMC 1310980 . PMID 16795165.  ^ Madden, Gregory J., ed. (2013). "APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis". Retrieved December 24, 2014.  ^ Crone-Todd, Darlene, ed. (2015). "Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice". Retrieved 2014-12-24.  ^ a b Skinner, BF (1976). About Behaviorism. New York: Random House, Inc. p. 18. ISBN 0-394-71618-3.  ^ Staats, Arthur W.; Staats, Carolyn K. (1963). Complex human behavior: A systematic extension of learning principles. New York, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.  ^ Staats, Arthur W. (1968). Learning, language, and cognition. New York, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.  ^ Moxley, R.A. (2004). "Pragmatic selectionism: The philosophy of behavior analysis" (PDF). The Behavior Analyst Today. 5 (1): 108–25. Retrieved 2008-01-10.  ^ Skinner, B.F. (1991). The Behavior of Organisms. Copley Pub Group. p. 473. ISBN 0-87411-487-X.  ^ Cheney, Carl D.; Ferster, Charles B. (1997). Schedules of Reinforcement (B.F. Skinner Reprint Series). Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group. p. 758. ISBN 0-87411-828-X.  ^ Commons, M.L. (2001). "A short history of the Society for the Quantitative Analysis of Behavior" (PDF). Behavior Analyst Today. 2 (3): 275–9. Retrieved 2008-01-10.  ^ Thornbury, Scott (1998). "The Lexical Approach: A journey without maps". Modern English Teacher. 7 (4): 7–13.  ^ Skinner, Burrhus Frederick (1957). Verbal Behavior. Acton, Massachusetts: Copley Publishing Group. ISBN 1-58390-021-7.  ^ Skinner, B.F. (1969). "An operant analysis of problem-solving": 133–57. ; chapter in Skinner, B.F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. p. 283. ISBN 0-13-171728-6.  ^ Chomsky, Noam; Skinner, B.F. (1959). "A Review of B.F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior". Language. 35 (1): 26–58. doi:10.2307/411334. JSTOR 411334.  ^ Kennison, Shelia M. (2013). Introduction to language development. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications. ISBN 9781483315324.  ^ Skinner, B.F. (1972). "I Have Been Misunderstood". Center Magazine (March–April): 63.  ^ MacCorquodale, K. (1970). "On Chomsky's Review of Skinner's VERBAL BEHAVIOR". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 13 (1): 83–99. doi:10.1901/jeab.1970.13-83. Retrieved 2008-01-10.  ^ Stemmer, N. (1990). "Skinner's verbal behavior, Chomsky's review, and mentalism". J Exp Anal Behav. 54 (3): 307–15. doi:10.1901/jeab.1990.54-307. PMC 1323000 . PMID 2103585.  ^ Thornbury, Scott (2006). An A-Z of ELT. Oxford: Macmillan. p. 24. ISBN 1405070633.  ^ Douglas Brown, H (2000). Principles of Language Learning and Teaching (Fourth ed.). White Plains: Longman/Pearson Education. pp. 8–9. ISBN 0-13-017816-0.  ^ Skinner, B.F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. p. 283. ISBN 0-13-171728-6.  ^ "Behaviorism - Learning Theories". Learning Theories. 2007-01-31. Retrieved 2017-08-04.  ^ "Classical and Operant Conditioning - Behaviorist Theories". Learning Theories. 2015-06-19. Retrieved 2017-08-04.  ^ "Ivan Pavlov". Retrieved 16 April 2012.  ^ Richard Gross, Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behaviour ^ Skinner, B.F (31 July 1981). "Selection by Consequences" (PDF). Science. 213 (4507): 501–4. Bibcode:1981Sci...213..501S. doi:10.1126/science.7244649. PMID 7244649. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 July 2010. Retrieved 14 August 2010.  ^ Fantino, E. (2000). "Delay-reduction theory—the case for temporal context: comment on Grace and Savastano (2000)". J Exp Psychol Gen. 129 (4): 444–6. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.129.4.444. PMID 11142857.  ^ Baum, W.M. (2003). "The molar view of behavior and its usefulness in behavior analysis". Behavior Analyst Today. 4: 78–81. doi:10.1037/h0100009. Retrieved 2008-01-10.  ^ Fantino, E.; Stolarz-Fantino, S.; Navarro, A. (2003). "Logical fallacies: A behavioral approach to reasoning". The Behavior Analyst Today. 4. p.116 (pp.109–117). Retrieved 2008-01-10.  ^ Dennett, D.C. "The Message is: There is no Medium". Tufts University. Archived from the original on 11 January 2008. Retrieved 2008-01-10.  ^ Dennett, Daniel (1981). Brainstorms: Philosophical Essays on Mind and Psychology. Bradford Books. MIT Press. p. 53. ISBN 978-0-262-54037-7. LCCN 78013723.  ^ Brown, Curtis (2001). "Behaviorism: Skinner and Dennett". Philosophy of Mind. San Antonio, TX: Trinity University.  ^ Mace, F. Charles (1994). "The significance and future of functional analysis methodologies". Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 27 (2): 385–392. doi:10.1901/jaba.1994.27-385. PMC 1297814 . PMID 16795830.  ^ Pelios, L.; Morren, J.; Tesch, D.; Axelrod, S. (1999). "The impact of functional analysis methodology on treatment choice for self-injurious and aggressive behavior". Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 32 (2): 185–95. doi:10.1901/jaba.1999.32-185. PMC 1284177 . PMID 10396771.  ^ Mace, F. Charles; Critchfield, Thomas S. (May 2010). "Translational research in behavior analysis: Historic traditions and imperative for the future". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 93 (3): 293–312. doi:10.1901/jeab.2010.93-293. PMC 2861871 . PMID 21119847.  ^ UK SBA. UK SBA. Retrieved on 2013-11-02. ^ Kellaway, Lucy (7 January 2015). "My team gets more excited by loo roll than business budgets: Work problems answered". London: Financial Times. p. 10. Retrieved 22 November 2015.  ^ Eyres, Harry (19 December 2009). "Peaks in a trough year: The Slow Lane". Financial Times. p. 22. Retrieved 22 November 2015.  ^ Stern, Stefan (5 August 2008). "Keep up motivation levels through long summer days". London: Financial Times. p. 12. Retrieved 22 November 2015.  ^ Skapinker, Michael (11 December 2002). "Human capitalism: Does treating workers well help business too? A PwC report provides some evidence". London: Financial Times. p. 22.  |access-date= requires |url= (help) ^ Skapinker, Michael (9 April 2013). "The 50 ideas that shaped business today". London: Financial Times. Retrieved 22 November 2015.  ^ Bolles, Richard N. (2013). What Color is Your Parachute. New York: Ten Speed Press. pp. 110–189. ISBN 978-1-60774-363-7.  ^ "Reinventing the deal; American capitalism" (417.8961). London: The Economist. 24 October 2015. pp. 21–24.  |access-date= requires |url= (help) ^ Hayes, S.C.; Barnes-Holmes, D. & Roche, B. (2001) Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. Kluwer Academic: New York. ^ Ward, Todd A.; Eastman, Raymond; Ninness, Chris (2009). "An Experimental Analysis of Cultural Materialism: The Effects of Various Modes of Production on Resource Sharing". Behavior and Social Issues. 18: 1–23. doi:10.5210/bsi.v18i1.1950.  ^ Jon Radoff (2011). "Gamification, Behaviorism and Bullsh$!". Radoff.com.  ^ Cao, Longbing (2010). "In-depth Behavior Understanding and Use: the Behavior Informatics Approach". Information Science. 180 (17): 3067–3085. doi:10.1016/j.ins.2010.03.025.  ^ Cao, Longbing; Joachims, Thorsten; et al. (2014). "Behavior Informatics: A New Perspective". IEEE Intelligent Systems. 29 (4): 62–80. doi:10.1109/mis.2014.60.  ^ Cao, Longbing; Yu, Philip (eds) (2012). Behavior Computing: Modeling, Analysis, Mining and Decision. Springer. ISBN 978-1-4471-2969-1. CS1 maint: Extra text: authors list (link) ^ Cao, Longbing; Motoda, Hiroshi; et al. (2013). Behavior and Social Computing. Springer. ISBN 978-3-319-04047-9.  ^ Friesen, N. (2005). Mind and Machine: Ethical and Epistemological Implications for Research. Thompson Rivers University, B.C., Canada. ^ Waldrop, M.M. (2002). The Dream Machine: JCR Licklider and the revolution that made computing personal. New York: Penguin Books. (pp. 139–40). ^ Chomsky, N (1959). "Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior". Language. 35: 26–58. doi:10.2307/411334.  Chomsky N. Preface to the reprint of A Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior. In: Jakobovits L.A, Miron M.S, editors. Readings in the psychology of language. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1967.


Further reading[edit] Baum, W.M. (2005) Understanding behaviorism: Behavior, Culture and Evolution. Blackwell. Cao, L.B. (2013) IJCAI2013 tutorial on behavior informatics and computing. Cao, L.B. (2014) Non-IIDness Learning in Behavioral and Social Data, The Computer Journal, 57(9): 1358–1370. Chiesa, Mecca (1994). "Radical Behaviorism: The Philosophy and the Science". Authors Cooperative, Inc. Cooper, John O., Heron, Timothy E., & Heward, William L. (2007). "Applied Behavior Analysis: Second Edition". Pearson. Ferster, C.B. & Skinner, B.F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Malott, Richard W. Principles of Behavior. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall, 2008. Print. Mills, John A., Control: A History of Behavioral Psychology, Paperback Edition, New York University Press 2000. Lattal, K.A. & Chase, P.N. (2003) "Behavior Theory and Philosophy". Plenum. Pierce, W. David & Cheney, Carl D. (2013). "Behavior Analysis and Learning: Fifth Edition". Psychology Press. Plotnik, Rod. (2005) Introduction to Psychology. Thomson-Wadsworth (ISBN 0-534-63407-9). Rachlin, H. (1991) Introduction to modern behaviorism. (3rd edition.) New York: Freeman. Skinner, B.F. Beyond Freedom & Dignity, Hackett Publishing Co, Inc 2002. Skinner, B.F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Skinner, B.F. (1945). "The operational analysis of psychological terms". Psychological Review. 52 (270–7): 290–4. doi:10.1037/h0062535.  Skinner, B.F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior (ISBN 0-02-929040-6) Online version. Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Skinner, B.F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Skinner, B.F. (31 July 1981). "Selection by Consequences" (PDF). Science. 213 (4507): 501–4. Bibcode:1981Sci...213..501S. doi:10.1126/science.7244649. PMID 7244649. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 July 2010. Retrieved 14 August 2010.  Klein, P. (2013) "Explanation of Behavioural Psychotherapy Styles". [1]. Staddon, J. (2014) The New Behaviorism, 2nd Edition. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. pp. xi, 1–282. Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177. (on-line). Watson, J.B. (1919). Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviorist. Watson, J.B. (1924). Behaviorism. Zuriff, G.E. (1985). Behaviorism: A Conceptual Reconstruction, Columbia University Press. LeClaire, J. and Rushin, J.P. (2010) Behavioral Analytics For Dummies. Wiley. (ISBN 978-0-470-58727-0).


External links[edit] Library resources about Behaviorism Resources in your library Resources in other libraries Wikiquote has quotations related to: Behaviorism Look up behaviorism in Wiktionary, the free dictionary. Automated Behavior analysis Behavior Informatics Graham, George. "Behaviorism". In Zalta, Edward N. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.  "Behaviorism". Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.  Dictionary of the History of Ideas: Behaviorism Books and Journal Articles On Behaviorism Wuerzburg University: behaviourism B.F. Skinner Foundation Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies Skinner's Theories APA Behaviour Analysis Association for Behavior Analysis Theory of Behavioral Anthropology (Documents No. 9 and 10 in English) California Association for Behavior Analysis Examining Learning From Multiple Perspectives by Michigan State University Association for Contextual Behavioral Science Behavior Analysis Online Tutorials v t e Psychology History Philosophy Portal Psychologist Basic psychology Abnormal Affective science Affective neuroscience Behavioral genetics Behavioral neuroscience Behaviorism Cognitive/Cognitivism Cognitive neuroscience Comparative Cross-cultural Cultural Developmental Differential Ecological Evolutionary Experimental Gestalt Intelligence Mathematical Neuropsychology Personality Positive Psycholinguistics Psychophysics Psychophysiology Quantitative Social Theoretical Applied psychology Anomalistic Applied behavior analysis Assessment Clinical Community Consumer Counseling Critical Educational Ergonomics Feminist Forensic Health Industrial and organizational Legal Media Military Music Occupational health Pastoral Political Psychometrics Psychotherapy Religion School Sport and exercise Suicidology Systems Traffic Methodologies Animal testing Archival research Behavior epigenetics Case study Content analysis Experiments Human subject research Interviews Neuroimaging Observation Qualitative research Quantitative research Self-report inventory Statistical surveys Psychologists William James (1842–1910) Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) Sigmund Freud (1856–1939) Edward Thorndike (1874–1949) Carl Jung (1875–1961) John B. Watson (1878–1958) Clark L. Hull (1884–1952) Kurt Lewin (1890–1947) Jean Piaget (1896–1980) Gordon Allport (1897–1967) J. P. Guilford (1897–1987) Carl Rogers (1902–1987) Erik Erikson (1902–1994) B. F. Skinner (1904–1990) Donald O. Hebb (1904–1985) Ernest Hilgard (1904–2001) Harry Harlow (1905–1981) Raymond Cattell (1905–1998) Abraham Maslow (1908–1970) Neal E. Miller (1909–2002) Jerome Bruner (1915–2016) Donald T. Campbell (1916–1996) Hans Eysenck (1916–1997) Herbert A. Simon (1916–2001) David McClelland (1917–1998) Leon Festinger (1919–1989) George Armitage Miller (1920–2012) Richard Lazarus (1922–2002) Stanley Schachter (1922–1997) Robert Zajonc (1923–2008) Albert Bandura (b. 1925) Roger Brown (1925–1997) Endel Tulving (b. 1927) Lawrence Kohlberg (1927–1987) Noam Chomsky (b. 1928) Ulric Neisser (1928–2012) Jerome Kagan (b. 1929) Walter Mischel (b. 1930) Elliot Aronson (b. 1932) Daniel Kahneman (b. 1934) Paul Ekman (b. 1934) Michael Posner (b. 1936) Amos Tversky (1937–1996) Bruce McEwen (b. 1938) Larry Squire (b. 1941) Richard E. Nisbett (b. 1941) Martin Seligman (b. 1942) Ed Diener (b. 1946) Shelley E. Taylor (b. 1946) John Anderson (b. 1947) Ronald C. Kessler (b. 1947) Joseph E. LeDoux (b. 1949) Richard Davidson (b. 1951) Susan Fiske (b. 1952) Roy Baumeister (b. 1953) Lists Counseling topics Disciplines Important publications Organizations Outline Psychologists Psychotherapies Research methods Schools of thought Timeline Topics Wiktionary definition Wiktionary category Wikisource Wikimedia Commons Wikiquote Wikinews Wikibooks v t e Philosophy of mind Theories Behaviorism (Radical) Biological naturalism Cognitive psychology Mind–body dualism Eliminative materialism Emergent materialism Emergentism Epiphenomenalism Functionalism Idealism Interactionism Materialism Monism Naïve realism Neurophenomenology Neutral monism Occasionalism Psychoanalysis Parallelism Phenomenalism Phenomenology Physicalism identity theory Property dualism Representational Solipsism Substance dualism Concepts Abstract object Artificial intelligence Chinese room Cognition Cognitive closure Concept Concept and object Consciousness Hard problem of consciousness Hypostatic abstraction Idea Identity Ingenuity Intelligence Intentionality Introspection Intuition Language of thought Materialism Mental event Mental image Mental process Mental property Mental representation Mind Mind–body problem Non-physical entity New mysterianism Pain Problem of other minds Propositional attitude Qualia Tabula rasa Understanding Zombie more... Related topics Metaphysics Philosophy of artificial intelligence / information / perception / self Portal Category Philosophers category Project Task Force Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Behaviorism&oldid=819489019" Categories: BehaviorismMind–body problemPhilosophy of psychologyPsychological theoriesTheory of mindHidden categories: Pages using citations with accessdate and no URLCS1 maint: Extra text: authors listAll articles with unsourced statementsArticles with unsourced statements from November 2012Articles with unsourced statements from January 2008Pages incorrectly using the quote templateArticles with a promotional tone from March 2015All articles with a promotional toneArticles lacking in-text citations from June 2016All articles lacking in-text citations


Navigation menu Personal tools Not logged inTalkContributionsCreate accountLog in Namespaces ArticleTalk Variants Views ReadEditView history More Search Navigation Main pageContentsFeatured contentCurrent eventsRandom articleDonate to WikipediaWikipedia store Interaction HelpAbout WikipediaCommunity portalRecent changesContact page Tools What links hereRelated changesUpload fileSpecial pagesPermanent linkPage informationWikidata itemCite this page Print/export Create a bookDownload as PDFPrintable version In other projects Wikimedia CommonsWikiquote Languages العربيةঅসমীয়াAsturianuAzərbaycancaБеларускаяБългарскиCatalàČeštinaDanskDeutschEestiΕλληνικάEmiliàn e rumagnòlEspañolEsperantoEuskaraفارسیFrançaisGaeilge한국어Հայերենहिन्दीHrvatskiBahasa IndonesiaÍslenskaItalianoעבריתქართულიҚазақшаКыргызчаLatinaLatviešuLietuviųMagyarМакедонскиമലയാളംBahasa MelayuNederlands日本語NorskNorsk nynorskOʻzbekcha/ўзбекчаPiemontèisTok PisinPolskiPortuguêsRomânăРусскийSimple EnglishسنڌيSlovenčinaSlovenščinaСрпски / srpskiSrpskohrvatski / српскохрватскиSuomiSvenskaTagalogதமிழ்ไทยТоҷикӣTürkçeУкраїнськаTiếng Việtייִדיש中文 Edit links This page was last edited on 9 January 2018, at 17:12. Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization. Privacy policy About Wikipedia Disclaimers Contact Wikipedia Developers Cookie statement Mobile view (window.RLQ=window.RLQ||[]).push(function(){mw.config.set({"wgPageParseReport":{"limitreport":{"cputime":"0.572","walltime":"0.690","ppvisitednodes":{"value":4604,"limit":1000000},"ppgeneratednodes":{"value":0,"limit":1500000},"postexpandincludesize":{"value":158668,"limit":2097152},"templateargumentsize":{"value":4481,"limit":2097152},"expansiondepth":{"value":15,"limit":40},"expensivefunctioncount":{"value":5,"limit":500},"entityaccesscount":{"value":0,"limit":400},"timingprofile":["100.00% 594.793 1 -total"," 48.85% 290.537 1 Template:Reflist"," 17.81% 105.905 25 Template:Cite_journal"," 15.78% 93.850 16 Template:Cite_book"," 11.04% 65.641 2 Template:Citation_needed"," 9.81% 58.353 2 Template:Fix"," 5.48% 32.613 9 Template:Cite_news"," 5.46% 32.460 4 Template:Category_handler"," 4.58% 27.213 1 Template:Quotation"," 4.08% 24.294 3 Template:ISBN"]},"scribunto":{"limitreport-timeusage":{"value":"0.297","limit":"10.000"},"limitreport-memusage":{"value":6290409,"limit":52428800}},"cachereport":{"origin":"mw1331","timestamp":"20180115202412","ttl":1900800,"transientcontent":false}}});});(window.RLQ=window.RLQ||[]).push(function(){mw.config.set({"wgBackendResponseTime":104,"wgHostname":"mw1327"});});


Behaviorists - Photos and All Basic Informations

Behaviorists More Links

BehavioralismBehavior Analysis (disambiguation)ReflexDepth PsychologyEdward ThorndikeLaw Of EffectReinforcementJohn B. WatsonIntrospectionB. F. SkinnerRadical BehaviorismIvan PavlovClassical ConditioningStimulus ControlOperant ConditioningRadical BehaviorismApplied Behavior AnalysisOrganizational Behavior ManagementAutismSubstance AbuseCognitive PsychologyCognitive Behavioral TherapyPhobiaPosttraumatic Stress DisorderMood DisordersJohn B. WatsonBehavior ModificationRadical BehaviorismB. F. SkinnerApplied Behavior AnalysisWillard Van Orman QuineMicroeconomicsEclecticismOccam's RazorPsychological BehaviorismClark L. HullEdward C. TolmanRadical BehaviorismExperimental Analysis Of BehaviorS–R TheoryTrial-and-errorSociety For Quantitative Analysis Of BehaviorVerbal BehaviorNoam ChomskyInnateness HypothesisLanguage AcquisitionRelational Frame TheoryRote LearningAudio-lingual MethodOperant ConditioningOperant ConditioningB.F. SkinnerBehaviour ModificationReinforcementPunishment (psychology)Operant Conditioning ChamberClassical ConditioningOperant ConditioningClassical ConditioningWikipedia:Citation NeededJohn B. WatsonBiologyNatural SelectionPhylogenyCultureMelioration TheoryHyperbolic DiscountingHoward RachlinRichard HerrnsteinRate Of ReinforcementLovePhilosophy Of MindNatural ScienceChemistryPhysicsEdmund FantinoPhilosophy Of LanguageAnalytic PhilosophyLudwig WittgensteinPhilosophical InvestigationsAlan TuringWikipedia:Citation NeededRudolf CarnapCarl HempelW.V. QuineGilbert RyleCategory MistakeDaniel DennettJoseph WeizenbaumELIZAWikipedia:What Wikipedia Is NotWikipedia:SpamWikipedia:External LinksWikipedia:Neutral Point Of ViewHelp:Maintenance Template RemovalBehavior ModificationApplied Behavior AnalysisAssociation For Behavior Analysis: InternationalOrganizational Behavior ManagementBehavior AnalysisRelational Frame TheoryAcceptance And Commitment TherapyAssociation For Contextual Behavioral ScienceWalden TwoMarvin HarrisGame DevelopmentBehavior InformaticsBehavior ComputingCognitive PsychologyCognitive NeuroscienceWikipedia:Citing SourcesWikipedia:External LinksWikipedia:Citing SourcesWikipedia:WikiProject Fact And Reference CheckWikipedia:When To CiteHelp:Maintenance Template RemovalCognitive RevolutionCognitive TherapyNoam ChomskyCognitive RevolutionAllen NewellHerbert A. SimonArtificial IntelligenceGeorge MandlerCognitive NeuroscienceDonald M. BaerAlbert BanduraDermot Barnes-HolmesVladimir BekhterevSidney W. BijouJacque FrescoEdwin Ray GuthrieSteven C. HayesRichard J. HerrnsteinClark L. HullBrian IwataAlan E. KazdinFred S. KellerJon Levy (behaviorist)Marsha M. LinehanOle Ivar LovaasNeal E. MillerO. Hobart MowrerCharles E. OsgoodIvan PavlovMurray SidmanB. F. SkinnerKenneth W. SpenceJ. E. R. StaddonEdward ThorndikeEdward C. TolmanJohn B. WatsonMontrose WolfAnimal TrainingAntecedent (behavioral Psychology)Behavior Analysis Of Child DevelopmentBehavioral Change TheoriesBehavioral EconomicsBehavioral MedicineBehavioral NeuroscienceCounterconditioningCriminologyDirect InstructionDog BehavioristEmergency PsychiatryEthologyFunctional Analysis (psychology)List Of Publications In PsychologyThe Logic Of Modern PhysicsMentalism (psychology)Models Of AbnormalityObservational LearningOperationalizationOrganizational Behavior ManagementPharmacologyPerceptual Control TheoryPositive Behavior SupportProfessional Practice Of Behavior AnalysisSocial Skills TrainingToken EconomyZebraBoxZoosemioticsBehavior TherapyBehavioral ActivationClinical Behavior AnalysisContingency ManagementDialectical Behavior TherapyDiscrete Trial TrainingExposure And Response PreventionExposure TherapyFunctional Analytic PsychotherapyHabit Reversal TrainingPivotal Response TreatmentRational Emotive Behavior TherapySystematic DesensitizationInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0962331147Digital Object IdentifierPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed CentralPubMed IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-394-71618-3B.F. SkinnerThe Behavior Of OrganismsInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-87411-487-XInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-87411-828-XInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/1-58390-021-7International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-13-171728-6Noam ChomskyDigital Object IdentifierJSTORInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/9781483315324Digital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed CentralPubMed IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/1405070633International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-13-017816-0International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-13-171728-6Science (journal)BibcodeDigital Object IdentifierPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierDaniel DennettInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-0-262-54037-7Library Of Congress Control NumberJournal Of Applied Behavior AnalysisDigital Object IdentifierPubMed CentralPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed CentralPubMed IdentifierJournal Of The Experimental Analysis Of BehaviorDigital Object IdentifierPubMed CentralPubMed IdentifierHelp:CS1 ErrorsInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-1-60774-363-7Help:CS1 ErrorsDigital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-1-4471-2969-1Category:CS1 Maint: Extra Text: Authors ListInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-3-319-04047-9Digital Object IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-534-63407-9Digital Object IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/0-02-929040-6Science (journal)BibcodeDigital Object IdentifierPubMed IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-0-470-58727-0Wikipedia:LIBRARYEdward N. ZaltaStanford Encyclopedia Of PhilosophyInternet Encyclopedia Of PhilosophyTemplate:PsychologyTemplate Talk:PsychologyPsychologyHistory Of PsychologyPhilosophy Of PsychologyPortal:PsychologyPsychologistBasic Science (psychology)Abnormal PsychologyAffective ScienceAffective NeuroscienceBehavioural GeneticsBehavioral NeuroscienceCognitive PsychologyCognitivism (psychology)Cognitive NeuroscienceComparative PsychologyCross-cultural PsychologyCultural PsychologyDevelopmental PsychologyDifferential PsychologyEcological PsychologyEvolutionary PsychologyExperimental PsychologyGestalt PsychologyIntelligenceMathematical PsychologyNeuropsychologyPersonality PsychologyPositive PsychologyPsycholinguisticsPsychophysicsPsychophysiologyQuantitative PsychologySocial PsychologyTheoretical PsychologyApplied PsychologyAnomalistic PsychologyApplied Behavior AnalysisPsychological TestingClinical PsychologyCommunity PsychologyConsumer BehaviourCounseling PsychologyCritical PsychologyEducational PsychologyHuman Factors And ErgonomicsFeminist PsychologyForensic PsychologyHealth PsychologyIndustrial And Organizational PsychologyLegal PsychologyMedia PsychologyMilitary PsychologyMusic PsychologyOccupational Health PsychologyPastoral PsychologyPolitical PsychologyPsychometricsPsychotherapyPsychology Of ReligionSchool PsychologySport PsychologySuicidologySystems PsychologyTraffic PsychologyList Of Psychological Research MethodsAnimal TestingArchival ResearchBehavioral EpigeneticsCase StudyContent AnalysisExperimental PsychologyHuman Subject ResearchInterview (research)NeuroimagingObservationQualitative Psychological ResearchQuantitative Psychological ResearchSelf-report InventorySurvey MethodologyList Of PsychologistsWilliam JamesIvan PavlovSigmund FreudEdward ThorndikeCarl JungJohn B. WatsonClark L. HullKurt LewinJean PiagetGordon AllportJ. P. GuilfordCarl RogersErik EriksonB. F. SkinnerDonald O. HebbErnest HilgardHarry HarlowRaymond CattellAbraham MaslowNeal E. MillerJerome BrunerDonald T. CampbellHans EysenckHerbert A. SimonDavid McClellandLeon FestingerGeorge Armitage MillerRichard LazarusStanley SchachterRobert ZajoncAlbert BanduraRoger Brown (psychologist)Endel TulvingLawrence KohlbergNoam ChomskyUlric NeisserJerome KaganWalter MischelElliot AronsonDaniel KahnemanPaul EkmanMichael Posner (psychologist)Amos TverskyBruce McEwenLarry SquireRichard E. NisbettMartin SeligmanEd DienerShelley E. TaylorJohn Robert Anderson (psychologist)Ronald C. KesslerJoseph E. LeDouxRichard DavidsonSusan FiskeRoy BaumeisterCategory:Psychology ListsList Of Counseling TopicsList Of Psychology DisciplinesList Of Important Publications In PsychologyList Of Psychology OrganizationsOutline Of PsychologyList Of PsychologistsList Of PsychotherapiesList Of Psychological Research MethodsList Of Psychological SchoolsTimeline Of PsychologyIndex Of Psychology ArticlesTemplate:Philosophy Of MindTemplate Talk:Philosophy Of MindPhilosophy Of MindRadical BehaviorismBiological NaturalismCognitive PsychologyMind–body DualismEliminative MaterialismEmergent MaterialismEmergentismEpiphenomenalismFunctionalism (philosophy Of Mind)IdealismInteractionism (philosophy Of Mind)MaterialismMonismNaïve RealismNeurophenomenologyNeutral MonismOccasionalismPsychoanalysisPsychophysical ParallelismPhenomenalismPhenomenology (philosophy)PhysicalismType PhysicalismProperty DualismMental RepresentationSolipsismSubstance DualismAbstract And ConcreteArtificial IntelligenceChinese RoomCognitionCognitive Closure (philosophy)ConceptConcept And ObjectConsciousnessHard Problem Of ConsciousnessHypostatic AbstractionIdeaIdentity (philosophy)IngenuityIntelligenceIntentionalityIntrospectionIntuitionLanguage Of Thought HypothesisMaterialismMental EventMental ImageMental ProcessMental PropertyMental RepresentationMindMind–body ProblemNon-physical EntityNew MysterianismPain (philosophy)Problem Of Other MindsPropositional AttitudeQualiaTabula RasaUnderstandingPhilosophical ZombieIndex Of Philosophy Of Mind ArticlesMetaphysicsPhilosophy Of Artificial IntelligencePhilosophy Of InformationPhilosophy Of PerceptionPhilosophy Of SelfPortal:Mind And BrainCategory:Philosophy Of MindCategory:Philosophers Of MindWikipedia:WikiProject PhilosophyWikipedia:WikiProject Philosophy/MindHelp:CategoryCategory:BehaviorismCategory:Mind–body ProblemCategory:Philosophy Of PsychologyCategory:Psychological TheoriesCategory:Theory Of MindCategory:Pages Using Citations With Accessdate And No URLCategory:CS1 Maint: Extra Text: Authors ListCategory:All Articles With Unsourced StatementsCategory:Articles With Unsourced Statements From November 2012Category:Articles With Unsourced Statements From January 2008Category:Pages Incorrectly Using The Quote TemplateCategory:Articles With A Promotional Tone From March 2015Category:All Articles With A Promotional ToneCategory:Articles Lacking In-text Citations From June 2016Category:All Articles Lacking In-text CitationsDiscussion About Edits From This IP Address [n]A List Of Edits Made From This IP Address [y]View The Content Page [c]Discussion About The Content Page [t]Edit This Page [e]Visit The Main Page [z]Guides To Browsing WikipediaFeatured Content – The Best Of WikipediaFind Background Information On Current EventsLoad A Random Article [x]Guidance On How To Use And Edit WikipediaFind Out About WikipediaAbout The Project, What You Can Do, Where To Find ThingsA List Of Recent Changes In The Wiki [r]List Of All English Wikipedia Pages Containing Links To This Page [j]Recent Changes In Pages Linked From This Page [k]Upload Files [u]A List Of All Special Pages [q]Wikipedia:AboutWikipedia:General Disclaimer



view link view link view link view link view link